Research article - (2025)24, 589 - 602 DOI: https://doi.org/10.52082/jssm.2025.589 |
Comparing Adapted Small-Sided Team Sports and Aerobic Exercise with or without Cognitive Games: Effects on Fitness and Cognition in Older Men |
Ana Filipa Silva1,2,![]() |
Key words: Walking football, walking basketball, walking handball, running, elderly, physical exercise |
Key Points |
|
|
|
Experimental approach to the problem |
To compare the effects of TS, TS+C, A, A+C, and a control group, a randomized, parallel, and controlled study design was implemented. Participants were randomly assigned to groups using a computer-generated block randomization method to ensure balanced allocation across the five study arms. The aim was to assess the impact of 12-week interventions on cognitive and physical fitness adaptations in older men. After recruitment, participants were randomly assigned to groups prior to the initial assessment. Randomization involved assigning each participant a code number, which was then allocated to a group through a random draw conducted by an independent person not involved in the study. Once randomized and allocated, participants remained in their assigned groups throughout the study. A convenience sampling strategy was used to enhance recruitment, targeting institutions already engaged with potential participants to increase the likelihood of enrollment. While this approach supported efficient recruitment, it may have introduced selection bias, potentially limiting the external validity and generalizability of the findings to broader populations. The study took place during the winter and spring months, with all interventions conducted in indoor facilities to facilitate adherence and ensure a consistent intervention process. All participants were evaluated during the week prior to the start of the 12-week intervention period and re-evaluated in the week following its completion. Participants were advised to engage only in the activity assigned within the study. Additionally, they were instructed to maintain their usual nutrition, hydration, and sleep habits throughout the intervention period, including those in the control group, who were asked not to initiate any new structured physical or cognitive activities during the 12 weeks. |
Participants |
From the initial pool, 59 volunteers were identified as suitable for the study after being assessed against the eligibility criteria. To be included, participants needed to: i) be 65 years of age or older; ii) possess no physical limitations that would prevent participation; iii) in line with the original validation by Nasreddine et al. (Nasreddine et al., This study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Instituto Politécnico de Viana do Castelo (protocol code: CECSVS2024/02/vi). Participants gave free informed consent, understanding their data would be protected and they could withdraw whenever they wished. |
Interventions |
Interventions were implemented over 12 weeks, with participants completing two 60-minute sessions per week, separated by 72 hours of rest. Conducted at partner facilities across, these sessions were guided by the research team, who also developed the prescribed aerobic and team sports training programs. Prior to the start of the intervention, instructors were selected based on their level of commitment and alignment with the researchers’ values. A preparatory training session was then conducted to ensure they understood the importance of standardizing the intervention process and were equipped to implement the training plan as designed by the research team. Full training protocols are detailed in Participants engaged in cognitive training (i.e., TS+C and A+C) have used the Fit4Alz software ( The control group did not receive any specific intervention during the 12-week study period. They continued their usual routines and were only required to attend the assessment sessions. |
Measurements and outcomes |
Data was collected in two distinct sessions both before and after the intervention. The first session focused on cognitive assessment, while the second was dedicated to physical fitness evaluation. All assessments were held indoors in a controlled environment during morning hours. A team of researchers administered the MoCA test in the initial session. Participants were organized into small groups of 4-5 and followed a pre-established sequence of activities. This session began with anthropometric measurements, followed by a warm-up, before proceeding to the six Senior Fitness Test components (39): i) chair stand; ii) arm curl; iii) chair sit-and-reach; iv) back scratch; v) 8-foot up-and-go; and vi) six-minute walk or 2-minute step-in-place test. It is important to note that no follow-up assessments were conducted after the post-intervention phase. This absence of longitudinal follow-up is a limitation of the study design, as it prevents evaluation of the long-term sustainability of the observed effects. |
MoCA |
As a cognitive assessment tool, the MoCA is both reliable and validated. It has showed sensitivity, particularly in comparison to alternative measures such as the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) (Freitas et al., |
Chair stand test |
To measure lower limb strength and endurance, a test was conducted using a stopwatch and a stabilized chair. The chair, roughly 43 cm in seat height and with a backrest, was secured against a wall or otherwise made immobile for participant safety. The participant positioned themselves with their back against the chair and feet flat on the floor, with an evaluator nearby providing additional chair stability. With arms crossed and middle fingers on shoulders, participants were instructed to stand completely and then sit down as many times as possible within a 30-second period after receiving the evaluator's signal. A single demonstration by the evaluator preceded the formal test to ensure clarity. |
Arm curl test |
Upper limb strength and endurance were evaluated using a stopwatch, an armrest-free chair, and hand weights (2.3 kg for women; 3.6 kg for men). Participants sat upright, dominant arm extended perpendicularly while gripping the weight. To ensure proper execution, an evaluator stabilized the upper arm. Upon signal, participants rotated their palm up, fully flexed the arm, and returned to extension, aiming for the highest number of repetitions within 30 seconds. Following a brief demonstration and practice, the test was performed once. |
Back Scratch test |
To evaluate flexibility, participants stood near an evaluator, who positioned themselves behind them. Participants then reached with their dominant hand from over their shoulder, down their back, while simultaneously bringing their other arm up from behind, aiming to touch or overlap their extended fingers. The evaluator ensured proper alignment of the middle fingers, preventing them from touching initially. Following two practice trials, participants completed two official test attempts. Scoring involved measuring the distance between the middle fingertips or the extent of their overlap, recorded to the nearest centimeter. A negative score (-) represented the shortest distance between fingers, whereas a positive score (+) indicated overlap. The top performance was used for assessment, with all observations of overlap or distance noted on the scoring sheet. |
Sit and reach test |
Lower limb flexibility was assessed using an armrest-free chair (around 43 cm high) and a 45 cm ruler. For stability, the chair was secured against a wall. Participants sat with their inguinal line parallel to the seat, one leg bent off the ground, and the other extended forward. An evaluator was present for support. Participants then leaned forward, sliding hands down the extended leg to touch their toes, while keeping a straight back. This position was held for two seconds, with instructions to straighten a bent knee if observed. Two trials were completed, and the best outcome was logged. |
Up and go test |
Measuring physical mobility, including speed, agility, and dynamic balance, was the goal of this test. It utilized a stopwatch, measuring tape, a cone (or marker), and a stabilized chair (about 43 cm high). The cone was set 2.44 meters from the chair, with 1.22 meters of clear surrounding space. Participants began seated upright, one foot slightly ahead of the other, with an evaluator close by for support. At the signal, they stood up, walked quickly around the cone, and returned to their seat. The timer started at the signal and paused upon reseating. After a demonstration, a single practice trial was allowed before two official attempts. The quickest (shortest) time achieved was used for the score. Participants received a reminder to walk quickly, not run, when navigating the cone and returning to the chair. |
6-min walk test |
Aerobic endurance was assessed using a stopwatch, measuring tape, cones, poles, chalk, and markers. For safety, chairs were positioned along the circuit. The 45-meter course, marked every 5 meters with chalk or tape, was in a well-lit, level area. Participants started with an evaluator ready to time them. Upon signal, they walked as fast as possible (no running), completing as many laps as they could in 6 minutes. Participants were allowed to rest as needed. The evaluator joined the course after the start to provide time updates. The 5-meter markings were key for this 6-minute walk test. |
Statistical procedures |
An a priori power analysis was conducted using G*Power (Version 3.1.9.7, Kiel University, Kiel, Germany(Faul et al., To analyze the effects of five groups on cognitive and physical fitness outcomes, a repeated measures ANOVA with a mixed design (time and group) was conducted using IBM SPSS Statistics (version 28.0; USA). Sphericity was evaluated using Mauchly's Test of Sphericity, and where violated, the Greenhouse-Geisser correction was applied. The assumption of normality for the residuals was assessed using the Shapiro-Wilk test (p > 0.05). In cases of significant main or interaction effects (p < 0.05), Bonferroni-corrected post hoc tests were performed to identify specific group differences or changes over time. Partial eta-squared (ηp2) values are reported as measures of effect size, with values of 0.01, 0.06, and 0.14 indicating small, medium, and large effects, respectively. |
|
|
A significant interaction between time and group was observed for the MoCA test (F(4,45) = 18.538; p < 0.001; ηp2 = 0.622), chair stand test (F(4,45) = 2.614; p = 0.048; ηp2 = 0.189), arm curl test (F(4,45) = 4.816; p = 0.003; ηp2 = 0.300), up and go test (F(4,45) = 11.997; p < 0.001; ηp2 = 0.516) and 6-minute walk test (F(4,45) = 5.627; p < 0.001; ηp2 = 0.333). The interaction effect on the MoCA test and the up and go test represent large effects, indicating substantial differences in performance changes between groups over time. Additionally, post hoc analyses revealed that the groups receiving combined interventions (TS+C and A+C) demonstrated significantly greater improvements than the control group in most outcomes, with large effect sizes (Cohen’s d > 0.8), reinforcing the practical relevance of these findings. |
MoCA |
At baseline, there was no significant difference in MoCA scores among the five groups, (F(4,45) = 2.295, p = 0.074, ηp2 = 0.169). Although not statistically significant, the effect size is above the threshold for a large effect, suggesting a potentially meaningful difference that may not have reached significance due to limited sample size. At post-intervention, there was a significant difference in MoCA scores among the groups (F(4,45) = 6.584, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.369), which represents a large effect size, indicating substantial between-group differences following the intervention. The specific post hoc group comparisons are illustrated in Within group comparisons revealed there was a significant increase in MoCA scores from pre- to post-intervention for the TS group (p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.693). The TS+C group also showed a significant increase in MoCA scores from pre- to post-intervention, (p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.763). There was a significant increase in MoCA scores from pre- to post-intervention for the A group (p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.340), reflecting a large effect, while the A+C group demonstrated a similarly large effect (p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.481). In contrast, the control group showed no significant change (p = 0.151), and the effect size was small (ηp2 = 0.045), suggesting minimal change in cognitive performance over time without intervention. |
Chair stand test |
At baseline, there was no significant difference in Chair Stand test scores among the five groups (F(4,45) = 0.203, p = 0.936, ηp2 = 0.018). This effect size falls within the small range, indicating minimal baseline differences across groups, which aligns with the non-significant result. At post-intervention, there was a significant difference in Chair Stand test scores among the groups (F(4,45) = 2.773, p = 0.038, ηp2 = 0.198), representing a large effect size and indicating meaningful differences between groups following the intervention. There was a significant increase in Chair Stand test scores from pre- to post-intervention for the TS group (p = 0.009, ηp2 = 0.143), which corresponds to a large effect, indicating meaningful improvements in lower-body strength. The TS+C group also showed a significant increase in Chair Stand test scores from pre- to post-intervention (p = 0.002, ηp2 = 0.187), also reflecting a large effect. The A group did not show a significant change (p = 0.636, ηp2 = 0.005), and the effect size was negligible, suggesting no meaningful improvement. The A+C group demonstrated a marginally significant increase (p = 0.050, ηp2 = 0.082), with a medium effect, indicating a modest but potentially meaningful gain. The control group showed no significant change (p = 0.573, ηp2 = 0.007), and the effect size was very small, consistent with the lack of intervention. |
Arm curl |
At baseline, there was no significant difference in arm curl scores among the five groups, (F(4,45) = 0.457, p = 0.767, ηp2 = 0.039). The effect size falls within the small range, suggesting only minimal variation between groups at baseline, consistent with the non-significant result. At post-intervention, there was no significant difference in arm curl scores among the groups (F(4,45) = 2.491, p = 0.056, ηp2 = 0.181), the effect size was large, indicating a potentially meaningful difference that may not have reached significance possibly due to limited statistical power. Detailed comparisons between the groups are presented in There was a significant increase in arm curl scores from pre- to post-intervention for the TS group (p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.276) and the TS+C group (p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.258), both representing large effect sizes. These results indicate strong improvements in upper-body strength for the team sports interventions. In contrast, the A group (p = 0.575, ηp2 = 0.007), A+C group (p = 0.431, ηp2 = 0.014), and Control group (p = 0.476, ηp2 = 0.011) did not show significant changes. The effect sizes for these three groups were negligible to small, suggesting no meaningful improvement in arm strength from pre- to post-intervention. |
Sit and reach test |
At baseline, there was no significant difference in sit and reach left scores among the five groups (F(4,45) = 0.280, p = 0.890, ηp2 = 0.024). This effect size falls within the small range, suggesting minimal variation in flexibility between groups at baseline. At post-intervention, there was no significant difference in sit and reach left scores among the groups (F(4,45) = 0.502, p = 0.734, ηp2 = 0.043). Detailed comparisons between the groups are presented in At baseline, there was no significant difference in sit and reach (right) scores among the five groups (F(4,45) = 1.469, p = 0.227, ηp2 = 0.115). While not statistically significant, this corresponds to a medium-to-large effect size, suggesting moderate baseline variability that did not reach significance, potentially due to sample size. At post-intervention, no significant difference was found either (F(4,45) = 1.069, p = 0.383, ηp2 = 0.087), but the effect size falls within the medium range, indicating a modest but not statistically conclusive variation in flexibility between groups. Detailed comparisons between the groups are presented in |
Back Scratch test |
At baseline, there was no significant difference in back scratch left scores among the five groups (F(4,45) = 0.187, p = 0.944, ηp2 = 0.016). This effect size falls in the small range, indicating negligible variation in upper-body flexibility between groups prior to the intervention. At post-intervention, there was no significant difference in back scratch left scores among the groups (F(4,45) = 0.144, p = 0.965, ηp2 = 0.013), with the effect size again being small, suggesting minimal change or differentiation between groups after the intervention. Detailed comparisons between the groups are presented in Moreover, at baseline, there was no significant difference in back scratch right scores among the five groups (F(4,45) = 0.147, p = 0.963, ηp2 = 0.013). This reflects a small effect size, indicating very little variability in upper-body flexibility across groups prior to the intervention. At post-intervention, there was no significant difference in back scratch right scores among the groups (F(4,45) = 0.270, p = 0.896, ηp2 = 0.023). Again, the effect size was small, suggesting limited differentiation between groups in flexibility outcomes after the intervention. Detailed comparisons between the groups are presented in |
Up and go test |
At baseline, there was no significant difference in Up and Go Test scores among the five groups (F(4,45) = 0.280, p = 0.890, ηp2 = 0.024). This corresponds to a small effect size, indicating very little variation in mobility and agility between groups before the intervention. At post-intervention, there was a significant difference in Up and Go Test scores among the groups (F(4,45) = 11.997, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.516). This represents a very large effect size, suggesting that the interventions had a substantial and meaningful impact on performance in the up and go test. There was a significant improvement in Up and Go Test scores from pre- to post-intervention for the TS group (p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.310), representing a large effect size and indicating a strong enhancement in agility and dynamic balance through team sports. The TS+C group also showed a significant improvement in Up and Go Test scores from pre- to post-intervention (p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.294), also reflecting a large effect size, suggesting that combining team sports with cognitive training produced substantial functional gains. The A group did not show a statistically significant change (p = 0.102, ηp2 = 0.058), with a small-to-medium effect size, indicating limited improvements from aerobic training alone. In contrast, the A+C group demonstrated a significant improvement (p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.217), with a large effect size, supporting the efficacy of the combined aerobic and cognitive training approach. Interestingly, the control group also showed a statistically significant change (p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.233), with a large effect size, however, this finding should be interpreted with caution, as the group did not undergo any structured intervention, and the observed change may reflect uncontrolled external factors or measurement variability. |
6-Minute Walk test |
At baseline, there was no significant difference in 6-minute walk test scores among the five groups (F(4,45) = 0.380, p = 0.822, ηp2 = 0.033), indicating a negligible effect size and similar initial physical endurance across groups. At post-intervention, a significant difference emerged among groups (F(4,45) = 5.627, p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.333), reflecting a large effect size and suggesting meaningful differences in walking endurance attributable to the interventions. The specific post hoc group comparisons are illustrated in There was a significant improvement in 6-minute walk test scores from pre- to post-intervention for the TS group (p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.693), both with very large effect sizes, indicating substantial gains in aerobic endurance following team sports-based interventions with or without cognitive components. The A group also showed a significant improvement (p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.340), corresponding to a large effect size, highlighting the benefits of aerobic training alone. The A+C group demonstrated significant improvement as well (p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.481), a large effect size, underscoring the effectiveness of combined aerobic and cognitive training. Conversely, the control group did not exhibit significant changes (p = 0.151, ηp2 = 0.045), reflecting a small effect size and suggesting no meaningful improvement without intervention. |
|
|
This study aimed to investigate the impact of a 12-week intervention involving team-based games and aerobic exercise, with and without cognitive stimulation, on slowing down cognitive decline. Results showed significant intervention-related effects on cognitive and physical performance outcomes. A strong interaction between time and group was observed for the MoCA test, chair stand, arm curl, up-and-go, and 6-minute walk tests, indicating that improvements varied meaningfully across the different interventions. Although all intervention groups showed progress over time, the TS and TS+C groups consistently exhibited the largest improvements compared to the control group, particularly in outcomes such as the MoCA, up-and-go, and 6-minute walk tests. Although the TS and TS+C groups generally showed greater improvements across several outcomes, these differences were not consistently statistically significant compared to the A and A+C groups, warranting cautious interpretation of any claims of superiority. While TS-based interventions appeared to offer added benefits relative to the control group, the lack of consistent between-group significance may be partly due to limited statistical power. Notably, large effect sizes were observed in outcomes such as MoCA scores and functional mobility (up and go, walk test), suggesting potential clinical relevance even in the absence of statistically significant differences. Significant time × group interactions were observed for cognitive function, strength, mobility, and walking distance, indicating that the type of intervention influenced these domains. In contrast, flexibility-related outcomes (sit-and-reach and back scratch tests) showed no significant interactions, likely reflecting the absence of targeted stretching or range-of-motion activities in the intervention protocols. The cognitive benefits observed in the team sports combined with cognitive training (TS+C) group align with growing evidence that physical activity enhances cognitive function in older adults, particularly when combined with cognitive stimulation. This combination appears to amplify gains by targeting executive functions, attention, and memory, supporting neuroplasticity and cognitive reserve. These effects are thought to be mediated by neurobiological mechanisms, including increased levels of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF), which supports synaptic plasticity and neuronal survival, improved cerebral vascularization enhancing brain perfusion, and reduced chronic inflammation (Kennedy et al., All physical interventions led to improvements in strength, mobility, and aerobic capacity, particularly in the TS and TS+C groups. These results highlight the multifaceted nature of team sports, which engage participants in dynamic and functionally relevant movements. The superior outcomes observed in these groups compared to the aerobic-only group may be attributed to the higher intensity, variety, and social engagement inherent to team-based activities. Research suggests that team sports and resistance training offer significant benefits for older adults, including improvements in physical function, psychological well-being, and quality of life (Pedersen et al., The superior performance of the TS and TS+C groups underscores the benefits of multicomponent interventions that combine physical, cognitive, and social stimulation. Team sports appear to offer a richer environment for promoting global health in older adults by integrating physical challenges, tactical decision-making, and interpersonal interactions. This multifaceted nature supports not only physical function and cognitive engagement but also social and psychological well-being. Although these aspects were not directly assessed in the present study, previous research indicates that participation in team sports is associated with enhanced social support, a stronger sense of belonging, and improved self-esteem in older adults (Andersen et al., These findings have important implications for the design of active aging programs. Incorporating team sports into community-based interventions may enhance adherence while promoting both physical and cognitive benefits, and the addition of structured cognitive exercises can further optimize these outcomes. Health professionals and policymakers should recognize the value of socially interactive, cognitively challenging physical activities as effective strategies to support healthy aging and delay physical or cognitive decline. Future research is needed to explore the long-term effects of combined physical and cognitive interventions using larger and more diverse samples. Such studies should investigate how different components, such as intensity, frequency, duration, and type of cognitive engagement, contribute to specific cognitive and physical outcomes. Additionally, examining individual differences in responsiveness, alongside the roles of social interaction and enjoyment, may help clarify how to best tailor interventions for older adults. Despite the promising results, several limitations should be considered when interpreting these findings. The relatively small sample size may have limited the statistical power to detect significant differences between intervention groups, particularly in outcomes where trends or large effect sizes were observed but failed to reach significance. As such, some potentially meaningful effects may have been underestimated or missed. The 12-week intervention period, while adequate to initiate improvements, may not have been sufficient to observe the full benefits of multicomponent programs, especially for domains like flexibility or cognitive reserve, which often require longer-term engagement. The absence of follow-up assessments further restricts our ability to determine the sustainability of the observed improvements over time, which is particularly relevant for cognitive and mobility-related outcomes. Thus, while the immediate effects are encouraging, caution is warranted in extrapolating these results to long-term functional or cognitive maintenance. In addition, possible self-selection bias and participant motivation (common in voluntary interventions) may have influenced adherence or responsiveness, especially in socially engaging formats like team sports. These factors may have contributed to the observed benefits in TS-based groups, independent of the intervention itself. Future studies should consider including follow-up evaluations at appropriate time points (such as 6 months post-intervention) to better assess the durability of cognitive and physical benefits. Motivation and self-selection bias may also have influenced the results. |
|
|
The present study demonstrated that interventions involving team sports, either alone or combined with cognitive training, led to significant improvements in both cognitive performance (MoCA) and physical function (Chair Stand, Arm Curl, Up and Go, 6-Minute Walk Test), when compared to a control group. Although the group engaged in aerobic training also showed improvements, these were of a smaller magnitude. No significant changes were observed in flexibility measures (Back Scratch and Chair Sit and Reach), regardless of the intervention. |
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS |
The authors have no conflict of interest to declare. This study received no specific grants, fellowships, or materials gifts from any funding agency in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors. The datasets generated during and/or analyzed during the current study are not publicly available but are available from the corresponding author who organized the study. |
AUTHOR BIOGRAPHY |
|
REFERENCES |
|
Email link to this article