|
|
|
ABSTRACT |
The 2D:4D ratio is a putative marker for prenatal testosterone and has the potential to explain variations in sport performance. To date there has been little research into the association between sporting performance, digit ratio and psychological variables. This study examined the relationship between 2D:4D and mental toughness, optimism, goal orientations, aggression, coping style and their association with sporting achievement. A post facto design was adopted. Participants consisted of an opportunity sample of 122 sports people: male (n =60) and female (n = 62) from a university in North East England. Following informed consent, a Vernier Caliper was used to measure digit ratio hand scans. Participants completed self-reports measures including, the Alternative Psychological Performance Inventory (Golby et al., 2008), Sport Mental Toughness Questionnaire (Sheard et al., 2009), Life Orientation Test-Revised (Scheier et al., 1994), Buss-Perry aggression (Buss-Perry, 1992) and 30 item coping style questionnaire (Joseph et al., 1995). MANOVA revealed significant gender differences in 2D:4D with males demonstrating lower ratios (Manning, 2002). The 2D:4D was found to differentiate eleven of the seventeen measured variables, including mental toughness scores (p < 0.001) and varying levels of sporting achievement i.e. international/national, regional and school levels (p< 0.001). Specifically, this difference was significant when comparing the highest (international/national) and lowest (leisure/school) groups. Perhaps there is a threshold for prenatal testosterone’s influence upon sporting ability. Further research is necessary to examine the subtle differences between competitors involved in different achievement levels. It is proposed that high prenatal levels of testosterone may contribute to the development of increased mental toughness, optimism, ego/task goal orientations in individuals, and hence aptitude towards sport. Findings lend support for the tentative claim that mental toughness may be partially biologically predetermined. Theoretical and practical implications are considered, along with limitations of the current study. |
Key words:
Digit ratio, sport performance, mental toughness, coping style, aggression.
|
Key
Points
- Increased prenatal testosterone exposure was associated with successful sport performance. There is a need to further consider psychological aspects related to sport performance and their potential relationship with 2D:4D.
- Mental toughness variables, optimism, goal orientation, hostility and coping style are related to levels of 2:4D.
- Perhaps with further substantiation, 2D:4D could provide a supporting objective measure, to complement existing psychometric instruments.
|
Success in sport is proposed to be determined by marginal psycho-physiological differences. Adaptive psychological variables, such as optimism (Norlander and Archer, 2002) and mental toughness (Golby and Sheard, 2004; Kuan and Roy, 2007), are increasingly recognized as important prerequisites to sporting success (Fletcher and Wagstaff, 2009). Research has recently considered the influence of biological factors on developing psychological constructs, i.e. (Golby and Sheard, 2006). Initial research exploring the biological basis of behavior has identified the organizational effects of prenatal testosterone on various adult psycho-physiological parameters (Neave et al., 2003). A putative marker for exposure to prenatal testosterone is the 2D:4D ratio (Honekopp et al., 2006). Testosterone influences the growth of the ring finger (4D), whereas oestrogen exposure stimulates the growth of the index finger (2D) (Manning, 2002). The ratio of the index finger to the ring finger (2D:4D) has been shown to be a sexually dimorphic trait. Specifically, males demonstrated a lower ratio, arguably due to increased prenatal testosterone exposure (Manning, 2002). It is suggested that high levels of prenatal testosterone may have a permanent masculinizing effect on human behavior, (Manning, 2002) which could potentially explain the relationship between sporting performance and 2D:4D (Honekopp et al., 2006). “Masculine” attributes arguably play an important role in sport performance, e.g., aggression. Sports performance has been found to negatively relate to 2D:4D in sports such as football, athletics, skiing, fencing, cross country running and general fitness (Manning, 2002; Honekopp et al., 2006). It appears that those exposed to increased levels of prenatal testosterone have greater sporting ability. However, the exclusively male samples included in these studies limit the ability to generalize findings. Only two studies focus on females, one identified a negative association between digit ratio and endurance running performance (Paul et al., 2006) and similarly, another study located a negative association between 2D:4D and fencing performance (Voracek et al., 2010). This link between digit ratio and sport performance is thought to be multidimensional. Significant associations have been widely noted between physiological parameters (e.g., effective cardiovascular system, physical fitness and visuo-spatial ability) and 2D:4D. Behavioral differences such as increased exercise frequency have been identified in those with low 2D:4D in comparison with high digit ratio (Honekopp et al., 2006). Surprisingly, only one study considered psychological variables in relation to levels of prenatal testosterone exposure. Tester and Campbell, 2007 assessed the relationship between 2D:4D, social potency and harm avoidance, although they reported no significant findings. As there is scarce research to guide the selection of relevant psychological constructs in relation to 2D:4D and sport, the current study rationalizes the inclusion of the selected variables with reference to their proposed importance in sport performance. One particular construct that may be deemed important in differentiating athletes’ performance is mental toughness (Kuan and Roy, 2007; Golby and Sheard, 2004). The ability to regulate emotion and imagery effectively, display commitment and determination, possess an uncontrollable desire to succeed, and an unshakeable confidence, are all characteristic of mentally tough individuals (Crust and Clough, 2005). A further feature of mental toughness is the ability to adapt to stressful conditions. Dealing successfully with competitive stressors requires effective coping strategies. Individuals tend to have a preferred coping style that can be defined as the individual’s tendency to respond and resolve problems with a particular style of action (Bolger, 1990). Three categories of response style: emotion (emotion regulation), task/approach (practical and logical solutions to eliminate or lessen the perceived stressor) and avoidance (avoiding consideration of the stressor) coping, were identified by Lazarus and Folkman, 1984. In exploring the relationship between coping style, optimism and mental toughness, Nicholls et al., 2008 identified a relationship between mental toughness and utilization of more approach coping strategies and fewer avoidance tactics. Furthermore, an association between mental toughness and optimism was also reported. Optimism is a disposition whereby individuals appraise potential outcomes positively (Burke et al., 2000). Optimism has been proposed to predict sporting achievement in sports such as cross country skiers and swimmers (Norlander and Archer, 2002). Several other psychological variables associated with sporting success have been related to optimism, including the direction of anxiety interpretation (Wilson et al., 2002). Specifically, athletes with a more optimistic disposition experienced increased facilitative anxiety perceptions (Wilson et al., 2002). Ntoumanis, 2001 proposed that an individual’s motivation to compete and succeed in sport is important in determining subsequent levels of application. One existing motivation theory is the achievement motivation theory (Duda and Hall, 2001) which emphasizes the importance of goal orientation. The model proposes two distinct ego (out- performing peers) and task (intrinsic involvement) orientated goal categories. Duda and Treasure, 2001 suggested that performers with adaptive, high task and ego goal orientations are better equipped to meet the demands of sport. However, research has suggested that task orientations are of greater benefit to sporting performance, since they promote self-control (Gano-Overway, 2008) and greater enjoyment (Spray et al., 1999). Aggression serves as the final psychological factor included in this study, and may be positively associated with prenatal testosterone levels (Bailey and Hurd, 2005). In a sporting context, aggression is related to the athletes’ tendency to “force action” (Kerr, 2004). Wilson, 1983 found that women with low 2D:4D were more assertive and aggressive. Several studies have shown a negative correlation between aggression and 2D:4D (Bailey and Hurd, 2005). Further evidence is necessary to investigate this relationship. This exploratory study was designed to examine the potential of varying levels of prenatal testosterone to differentiate individuals’ levels of mental toughness, preference of coping style, optimism, ego and task goal orientations and aggression, which are considered to be important prerequisites of sporting excellence.
ParticipantsThe participants in this study were an opportunity sample of 122 (male = 60 and females = 62) sports people from a North Eastern university categorized as 18-25 years (n = 90) and 25+ years (n = 32), from all levels of sporting achievement: International/national (n = 23), Regional (n = 43), School/recreational (n = 56). Participants included competitors from a range of sports, including swimming and climbing, and had between four and thirty years’ experience of competition.
Measures Mental toughnessThe Alternative Psychological Performance Inventory (PPI-A, Golby et al., 2007) was used to measure mental toughness. The questionnaire obtains an overall toughness score, and 4 sub scale scores: self-belief, determination, positive cognition and visualization. Responses are given on a five point Likert scale, which ranges from ’almost always’ to ’almost never’. Collectively satisfying absolute and incremental fit index benchmarks, the inventory possesses satisfactory psychometric properties, with adequate reliability and convergent and discriminant validity (Golby et al., 2007). The Sport Mental Toughness Questionnaire (SMTQ, Sheard et al., 2009) was used as a secondary measure of mental toughness. The questionnaire yields a total mental toughness figure, and 3 subscale scores: confidence, constancy and control. There is preliminary support for the factor structure, reliability and validity of the measure (Sheard et al., 2009).
AggressionThe Buss-Perry scale (Buss and Perry, 1992) provides an overall value of aggression and four sub-scale measures: Physical aggression (9 items), Verbal aggression (6 items), Anger (6 items) and Hostility (8 items). Statements are rated on a seven point Likert scale, ranging from ’extremely characteristic of me’ to ’extremely uncharacteristic of me’. The measure has acceptable psychometric properties (Buss and Perry, 1992) and has been previously administered to sports persons (LeMieux et al., 2002).
Optimism scaleThe Revised Life-Orientation Test (Scheier et al., 1994) comprises of three positive items, three negative items and four filler items. Statements are rated on a five point Likert scale, ranging from ’strongly agree’ to ’strongly disagree’. Scores range from 6-30, with a greater score representing higher optimism. Studies have shown the LOT-R to have acceptable validity and internal consistency (Smith, 2003). It is also appropriate in the assessment of athletes (Czech et al., 2002).
Coping strategiesJoseph et al., 1995 selected thirty coping style items, which were also used in this study. Style can be grouped into three categories: emotion (10 items), problem (10 items) and avoidance coping (10 items) (Endler and Parker, 1990). Statements are rated on a four point Likert scale, ranging from ’I do this a lot’ to ’I never do this’. The score for each conceptual grouping can range from 10 to 40; overall scores ranging from 30 to 120. Psychometric properties of this measure are adequate (Joseph et al., 1995). Support is given to the utilization of this tool with competitive athletes (Nicholls et al., 2008).
Goal orientationsThe Task and Ego Orientation in Sport Questionnaire (Duda and Nicholls, 1992) contains thirteen items: six indicating ego orientation and seven showing a task orientation. Items were rated on a five point Likert scale, ranging from ’strongly agree’ to ’strongly disagree’. Scores for ego orientation range from 6 to 30 and task orientation range from 7 to 35. The validity and internal reliability of this measure are deemed adequate (Ebbeck and Becker, 1994).
Digit ratio measurementDigit ratio was measured using Vernier Calipers (Fink et al., 2006) accurate to 0.01 mm, which was grouped into low (n = 61) and high (n = 61) categories. Mean digit ratio for males is 0.96 (Manning, 2002), standard deviation is 0.04. The low group (0.90-0.96) is one and a half standard deviations below the mean. The high group (0.96-1.04) is two standard deviations above the mean.
ProcedureFollowing approval from the University Ethics Committee, requirements of the study were explained to participants before they gave signed consent to take part in the research. The confidentiality of results was explained to participants, who then completed the battery of questionnaires. They were finally required to provide a digital hand scan of both hands, which were computed using Vernier Calipers (Fink et al., 2006).
Data analysesTo explore the nature of the relationship between 2D:4D digit ratio and psychological variables, digit ratio was dichotomised using a mean split. Sport achievement level (international/national, regional, school and leisure) and digit ratio (high and low) group differences for each measured psychological characteristic were explored using a multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA). Alpha was set at 0.05. Recent studies have been criticized for including several measures of digit ratio within a single analysis (left/right hand) as it increases the probability of type 1 error (Voracek et al., 2006a; 2006b). Whilst the current research acknowledges this argument, Bonferroni correction compensates for this potential error. For the post-hoc tests, significance was set at p < 0.025.
MMeans and standard deviations of all measured variables are presented in Table 1. There was a significant multivariate effect for right hand digit ratio, Wilks √ = 0.581, F (1, 121) = 4.04, p <0.001, partial n2 = 0.419, with significant differences observed in eleven of the seventeen dependant variables. This effect size is medium (Cohen, 1992) and is comparable to those reported by previous 2D:4D studies outside of sport (Grimbos et al., 2010). Specifically, there were significant group differences in: determination F (1, 121) = 23.22, p < 0.001, partial n2 = 0.162; self-belief F (1, 121) = 20.75, p < 0.001, partial n2 = 0.147; positive cognition F (1, 121) = 34.72 p < 0.001, partial n2 = 0.224; visualization F (1, 121) = 44.55 P < 0.001, partial n2 = 0.271; confidence F (1, 121) = 17.68 p < 0.001, partial n2 = 0.128; constancy F (1, 121) = 7.604 p < 0.01, partial n2 = 0.060; control F (1, 121) = 18.069 p < 0.001, partial n2 = 0.131; optimism F (1, 121) = 9.989 p < 0.01, partial n2 = 0.077; ego goal orientation F (1, 121) = 4.013 P < 0.05, partial n2 = 0.032; task goal orientation F (1, 121) = 9.654 p < 0.01, partial n2 = 0.074 where those with a lower digit ratio scored higher than those with high 2D:4D. A significant group difference was located in relation to hostility F (1, 121) = 15.095 p < 0.001, partial n2 = 0.112, where those with high 2D:4D reported significantly greater levels. All effect sizes are small to moderate in size, with positive cognition and visualization demonstrating the largest effect sizes of all measured variables. Despite the small effect sizes located in this study, this knowledge is arguably valuable due to the subtlety of the psychological differences considered in this research (Cohen, 1992). There was a larger significant multivariate effect for left hand digit ratio, Wilks √ = 0.522, F (1, 121) = 3.98, P< 0.001, partial n2 = 0.388. All significant effects shown for right hand are identical for left hand ratio. There was a significant medium multivariate effect for sporting level achievement, Wilks √ = 0.00, F (1, 121) = 3.505, p < 0.001, partial n2 = 0.366, with significant differences observed in 14 of the dependant variables (digit ratio was utilised as a dependant variable also). Means and standard deviations of all variables displaying significant findings are contained in Table 2, and post-hoc analysis results are presented in Table 3.
This paper aimed to explore the relationship between 2D:4D and several psychological variables, selected for their relevance to sporting success. It is the first study, within 2D:4D literature, to focus on this particular combination of variables and to utilize a sample of mixed gender. Sexual dimorphism was observed in digit ratio, as expected (Manning, 2002), with males demonstrating lower values. The main findings substantiate previous research; those exposed to high prenatal testosterone possess greater aptitude within sport (Honekopp et al., 2006; Manning, 2002; Manning and Taylor, 2001; Voracek et al., 2010). This difference was significant when comparing the highest (international/national) and lowest (leisure/school) groups; perhaps there is a threshold for prenatal testosterone’s influence upon sporting ability. This finding extends previous research which merely identified a negative relationship between highest sport achievement level and digit ratio (Honekopp et al., 2006). Future research may wish to explore the variances in digit ratio between differing competitive levels further. The psychological differences identified among performers, competing at different sporting levels in this study, may illuminate the nature of this relationship. Significant differences were identified in relation to all measured mental toughness subcomponents. Specifically, those competing at higher levels (international/national), and with low 2D:4D, scored higher on the subscales of self-belief (SMTQ), visualisation (SMTQ), confidence (PPI-A), control (PPI-A), constancy (PPI-A), determination (SMTQ) and positive cognition (SMTQ) than those with high 2D:4D and competing at lower levels (leisure/school). This provides further support for the claim that these characteristics are important in sport performance (Crust, 2008; Golby and Sheard, 2004; Gucciardi et al., 2008), and also highlights the potential of prenatal testosterone in partially explaining their development. However, it is important to consider the possibility that the present results could be due to a remarkable coincidence, and further evidence is necessary before firm conclusions can be drawn. Furthermore, athletes participating in the lower level of sporting achievement (school/leisure), selected emotion focused coping as the preferred style, significantly more than regional level individuals. Research has provided strong evidence for the claim that greater self-confidence is associated with an increased perception of influence over one’s life outcomes (Moritz et al., 2000).This sense of empowerment, coupled with increased positive cognition, could relate to higher levels of emotional stability (Gucciardi et al., 2008). This would indicate less use of emotion focused strategies, which is characteristic of the mentally tough person (Nicholls et al., 2008). However, no differences were identified in individuals’ use of avoidance and approach coping strategies. It is speculated that individuals performing at a high level have the ability to select coping strategies according to the stressor presented at that time. Future research may wish to address this claim. Findings of the present study also show that those people who compete at an international/national standard and with low 2D:4D scored significantly higher on the subcomponents of determination (SMTQ) and visualization (SMTQ) than those competing at all lower levels and with high 2D:4D. Perhaps those competing at an elite level are likely to be exposed to visualization and imagery techniques more frequently than their non-elite counterparts (MacIntyre and Moran, 2007). They may possessgreater determination, because of the enhanced work ethic necessary to succeed (Holland et al., 2010). Furthermore, the competitive nature of sport and constant performance comparison among athletes is likely to increase the use of ego goals (Stornes and Ommundsen, 2004). Results found that individuals with a low 2D:4D reported high scores of optimistic disposition than those with high 2D:4D. Significant differences on the dispositional optimism scale were observed in relation to sporting level of achievement. Specifically, those competing at higher levels reported a greater optimistic disposition. This finding appears to support previous research, which suggests that optimism level may be an important factor in determining the level of effort invested to achieve goals (Nicholls et al., 2008) and is a significant predictor of success (Norlander and Archer, 2002). Interestingly, this study’s findings in relation to self-reported aggression scores appear incongruent with previous 2D:4D research. Several studies had noted a negative relationship between all subscales of aggression and 2D:4D, which was most recently replicated by Bailey and Hurd, 2005. Whereas, the present study found that individuals with low 2D:4D, and currently competing in higher levels of sport, reported significantly lower hostility. However, this finding is consistent with evidence from sport psychology: hostility was shown to be positively related to risk of injury (Galambos et al., 2005) and negatively associated with the number of training hours undertaken in martial arts (Daniels and Thornton, 1992). Further research may wish to explore the exact mechanisms underlying this relationship. The current study serves as preliminary research into the associations between 2D:4D and several psychological characteristics thought to be related to successful sporting achievement. Present findings highlight the scope for future research in this area, along with the tentative proposal that prenatal testosterone exposure may partially explain adult sporting success. The prospect of developing objective biological measures, such as 2D:4D ratio, to support self-reported measures of assessment, could significantly advance issues with measurement validity and reliability. Additionally, the potential for fabricated or bias responses on self-report measures could be reduced with the addition of biological indices (Chalabaev et al., 2009). Limitations of the present research include its heterogeneous sample; addressing similar research questions within specific sports and levels may provide a more detailed insight. However, it should be noted that the psychological measurements selected for use in the present study may require further assessment and development i.e. mental toughness questionnaires (Crust, 2008) and the Task and Ego Goal Orientation in Sport Questionnaire (Lane et al., 2005). Until such issues are addressed, perhaps future research may wish to employ other sound psychometric measurements in the consideration of 2D:4D and psychological variables relevant to sport. Notwithstanding, the present study shows a clear link between sporting ability and 2D:4D ratio which is worthy of further research.
The current paper provides initial support for an association between prenatal testosterone levels and mental toughness, optimism, goal orientations, coping strategies and hostility. Findings tentatively suggest that the mentioned psychological characteristics may be partially biologically predetermined.
|
AUTHOR BIOGRAPHY |
|
Jim Golby |
Employment: Head of Research in Sport and Exercise Psychology, Teesside University, Middlesbrough, England, United Kingdom. |
Degree: M.A, Mphil, PhD, CPsychol. |
Research interests: Psychology of elite performance and psychometric assessment in sport and exercise, HPA axis and psychological functioning, exercise and obesity; and exercise and eating disorders. |
E-mail: J.golby@tees.ac.uk |
|
|
Jennifer Meggs |
Employment: Lecturer in Sport and Exercise Psychology, Teesside University, Middlesbrough, England, United Kingdom. |
Degree: Bsc. (Hons.). PhD student. |
Research interests: The psychology of elite performance, mental toughness, self-concept structure in sport, identity in sport, the HPA axis and its relationship with sport performance. |
E-mail: E5049244@tees.ac.uk |
|
|
|
REFERENCES |
Austin E.J., Manning J.T., McInroy K., Mathews E. (2002) A preliminary investigation of the associations between personality, cognitive ability and digit ratio. Elsevier, Personality and Individual Differences 33, 1115-1124.
|
Bailey A.A., Hurd P.L. (2005) Finger length ratio (2D:4D) correlates with physical aggression in men but not in women. Elsevier, Biological Psychology 68, 215-222.
|
Bolger N. (1990) Coping as a personality process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 59, 525-537.
|
Bull S.J., Shambrook C.J., James W., Brooks J.E. (2005) Towards an understanding of mental toughness in elite English cricketers. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology 17, 209-227.
|
Burke K.L., Joyner A.B., Czech D.R., Wilson M.J. (2000) An investigation of concurrent validity between two optimism/pessimism questionnaires: the life orientation test-revised and the optimism/pessimism scale. Current Psychology 19, 129-136.
|
Buss A.H., Perry M. (1992) The Aggression Questionnaire. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 63, 452-459.
|
Chalabaev A., Major B., Cury F., Sarrazin P. (2009) Physiological markers of challenge and threat mediate the effects of performance-based goals on performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 45, 991-994.
|
Cohen J. (1992) A power primer. Psychology Bulletin 112, 155-159.
|
Connaughton D., Wadey R., Hanton S., Jones G. (2008) The development and maintenance of mental toughness: perceptions of elite performers. Journal of Sports Science 26, 83-95.
|
Connaughton D., Wadey R., Hanton S., Jones G. (2008) The development and maintenance of mental toughness: perceptions of elite performers. Journal of Sports Science 26, 83-95.
|
Crust L. (2008) A review and conceptual re-examination of mental toughness: Implications for future researchers. Elsevier, Personality and Individual Differences 45, 576-583.
|
Crust L., Clough P.J. (2005) Relationship between mental toughness and physical endurance. Perceptual and Motor Skills 100, 192-194.
|
Czech D.R., Burke K.L., Joyner A.B., Hardy C.J. (2002) An examination of NCAA Division 1 athlete’s optimism, pessimism and sport orientation levels. International Sports Journal 6, 136-146.
|
Daniels K., Thornton E. (1992) Length of training, hostility and the martial arts: a comparison with other sporting groups. British Journal of Sport Medicine 26, 118-120.
|
Duda J.L., Hall H. (2001) In: Handbook of Sport Psychology. Achievement goal theory in sport: Recent extensions and future directions. NY. Wiley.
|
Duda J.L., Nicholls J.G. (1992) Dimensions of achievement motivation in schoolwork and sport. Journal of Educational Psychology 84, 290-299.
|
Duda J.L., Treasure D.C., Williams J. (2001) Applied Sport Psychology. Toward optimal motivation in sport: Fostering athletes’ competence and sense of control. In. Mayfield Press.
|
Ebbeck V., Becker S.L. (1994) Psychosocial predictors of goal orientations in youth soccer. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport 65, 355-362.
|
Endler N.S., Parker J.D.A. (1990) State and trait anxiety: depression and coping styles. Australian Journal of Psychology 42, 207-229.
|
Fink B., Neave N., Laughton K., Manning J.T. (2006) Second to fourth digit ratio and sensation seeking. Personality and Individual Differences 41, 1253-1262.
|
Fletcher D., Wagstaff C. R. D. (2009) Organisational psychology in elite sport: its emergence, application and future. Psychology of Sport and Exercise 10, 427-434.
|
Galambos A., Terry P.C., Moyle G.M., Locke A. (2005) Psychological predictors of injury among elite athletes. British Journal of Sports Medicine 39, 351-354.
|
Gano-Overway L.A. (2008) The Effect of Goal Involvement on Self Regulatory Processes. International Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology 6, 132-.
|
Golby J., Sheard M. (2004) Mental toughness and hardiness at different levels of rugby league. Elsevier: Personality and Individual Differences 37, 933-942.
|
Golby J., Sheard M. (2006) The relationship between genotype and positive psychological development in National-level swimmers. European Psychologist 11, 143-148.
|
Golby J., Sheard M., Van Wersch A. (2007) Evaluating the Factor Structure of the Psychological Performance Inventory. Perceptual and Motor Skills 105, 309-325.
|
Golby J., Sheard M., Van Wersch A. (2008) Evaluating the factor structure of the psychological performance inventory. Perceptual and Motor Skills 105, 309-325.
|
Gould D., Dieffenbach K., Moffett A. (2002) Psychological talent and its development in Olympic champions. Journal of Applied Sport 14, 177-210.
|
Grimbos T., Dawood K., Burriss R.P., Zucker K.J., Puts D.A. (2010) Sexual orientation and the second to fourth finger length ratio: A meta-analysis in men and women. Behavioral Neuroscience 124, 278-287.
|
Gucciardi D., Gordon S., Dimmock J. (2008) Towards an understanding of mental toughness in Australian football. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology 20, 261-281.
|
Holland M. J. G., Woodcock C., Cumming J., Duda J. L. (2010) Mental qualities and employed mental techniques of young elite team sport athletes. Journal of Clinical Sport Psychology 4, 19-38.
|
Honekopp J., Manning J.T., Muller C. (2006) Digit ratio (2D:4D) and physical fitness in males and females: Evidence for effects of prenatal androgens on sexually selected traits. Elsevier, Hormones and Behaviour 49, 545-549.
|
Joseph S., Williams R., Yule W. (1995) Psychosocial perspectives on post-traumatic stress. Clinical Psychology Review 15, 515-544.
|
Kerr J.H. (2004) Rethinking aggression and violence in sport. Routledge.
|
Kuan G., Jolly R. (2007) Goal profiles, mental toughness and its influence on performance outcomes among Wushu athletes. Journal of Sports Science 6, 28-33.
|
Lane A. M., Nevill A. M., Bowes N., Fox K. R. (2005) Test-retest stability of the Task and Ego Orientation Questionnaire. Research Quarterly in Exercise and Sport 76, 339-46.
|
Lazarus R.S., Folkman S. (1984) Stress, appraisal and coping. NY. Springer.
|
LeMieux P., McKelvie S.J., Stout D. (2002) Athletic Insight Online Journal of Sport Psychology. http://www.athleticinsight.com/Vol4Iss3/SelfReportedAggression.htm
|
Loehr J.E. (1986) Mental toughness training for sport: achieving athletic excellence. New York. Plume.
|
MacIntyre T.E., Moran A.P. (2007) A qualitative investigation of imagery use and meta-imagery processes among the canoe slalom competitors. Journal of Imagery Research in Sport and Physical Activity 2, 1-23.
|
Manning J.T. (2002) Digit ratio: a pointer to fertility, behaviour and health. Rutgers University Press.
|
Manning J.T., Taylor R.P. (2001) Second to fourth digit ratio and male ability in sport: implications for sexual selection in humans. Evolution and Human Behaviour 22, 61-69.
|
Moritz S.E., Feltz D.L., Fahrbach K.R., Mack D.E. (2000) The relation of self-efficacy measures to sport performance: A meta-analytic review. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport 71, 280-294.
|
Neave N., Laing S., Fink B., Manning J. T. (2003) Second to fourth digit ratio, testosterone and perceived male dominance. The Royal Society 270, 2167-2172.
|
Newton M., Duda J. (1993) The relationship of task and ego orienttation to performance-cognitive content, affect and attributions in bowling. Journal of Sport Behaviour 16, 124-128.
|
Nicholls A.R., Polman R.C.J., Levy A.R., Backhouse S.H. (2008) Mental toughness, optimism, pessimism, and coping among athletes. Elsevier, Personality and Individual Differences 44, 1182-1192.
|
Norlander T., Archer T. (2002) Predicting performance in ski and swim championships: effectiveness of mood, perceived exertion and dispositional optimism. Perceptual Motor Skills 94, 153-164.
|
Ntoumanis M. (2001) A self determination approach to the understanding of motivation in physical education. British Journal of Educational Psychology 71, 225-242.
|
Paul S.N., Kato B.S., Hunkin J L., Vivekanandan S., Spector T.D. (2006) The Big Finger: the second to fourth digit ratio is a predictor of sporting ability in women. British Journal of Sport Medicine 40, 981-983.
|
Scheier M.F., Carver C.S. (1985) Optimism, coping and health: Assessment and implications of generalised outcome expectancies. Health Psychology 4, 219-247.
|
Scheier M.F., Carver C.S., Bridges M.W. (1994) Distinguishing optimism from neuroticism (and trait anxiety, self-mastery and self-esteem). A re-evaluation of the Life Orientation Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 67, 1063-1078.
|
Seligman M.E.P. (1990) Learned Optimism. New York. Pocket Books.
|
Sheard M., Golby J. (2006) Effect of psychological skills training program on swimming performance and positive psychological development. International Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology 2, 7-24.
|
Sheard M., Golby J., Van-Wersch A. (2009) Progress toward construct validation of the Sports Mental Toughness Questionnaire (SMTQ). European Journal of Psychological Assessment 25, 186-193.
|
Smith N. (2003) The revised life orientation test (LOT-R). ALSWH Data Dictionary Supplement, Section 2, Core Survey Dataset , -.
|
Spray C.M., Biddle S.J.H., Fox K.R. (1999) Achievement goals, beliefs about causes of success and reported emotion in post-16 physical education. Journal of Sports Science 17, 213-219.
|
Stornes T., Ommundsen Y. (2004) Achievement goals, motivational climate and sportspersonship: a study of young handball players. Scandinavian Journal of Educational research 48, 205-221.
|
Tester N., Campbell A. (2007) Digit ratio and sporting prowess: What are the psychological mediators?. Journal of Personality 75, 663-678.
|
Voracek M., Dressler S.G. (2006a) High digit ratio (2D:4D) in Danish men: a question of measurement method?. Human Reproduction 21, 1329-1331.
|
Voracek M., Manning J.T., Dressler S.G. (2006b) Repeatability and interobserver error of digit ratio (2D:4D) measurements made by experts. American Journal of Human Biology 19, 142-146.
|
Voracek M., Reimer B., Dressler G. (2010) Digit ratio (2D:4D) predicts sporting success among female fencers independent from physical, experience, and personality factors. Scandinavian Journal of Medicine & Science in Sports 20, 853-860.
|
Weinberg R.S., Gould D. (2006) Foundations of Sport and Exercise Psychology. Human Kinetics.
|
Wilson G.D. (1983) Finger length as an index of assertiveness in women. Personality and Individual Differences 4, 111-112.
|
Wilson G.S., Raglin J.S., Pritchard M.E. (2002) Optimism, pessimism, and pre competition anxiety in college athletes. Elsevier, Personality and Individual Differences 32, 893-902.
|
|
|
|
|
|
|